Analyse synthesise apply

This level helps us recall foundational or factual information: names, dates, formulas, definitions, components, or methods. Understanding means that we can explain main ideas and concepts and make meaning by interpreting, classifying, summarizing, inferring, comparing, and explaining. Application allows us to recognize or use concepts in real-world situations and to address when, where, or how to employ methods and ideas. Analysis means breaking a topic or idea into components or examining a subject from different perspectives.

Analysis helps reveal the connections between facts. Synthesizing means considering individual elements together for the purpose of drawing conclusions, identifying themes, or determining common elements. Evaluating means making judgments about something based on criteria and standards. This requires checking and critiquing an argument or concept to form an opinion about its value. This paper identifies the particular issues that relate especially to reviewing qualitative research systematically and then to describing the activity of thematic synthesis in detail.

When searching for studies for inclusion in a 'traditional' statistical meta-analysis, the aim of searching is to locate all relevant studies. Failing to do this can undermine the statistical models that underpin the analysis and bias the results. However, Doyle [[ 39 ], p] states that, "like meta-analysis, meta-ethnography utilizes multiple empirical studies but, unlike meta-analysis, the sample is purposive rather than exhaustive because the purpose is interpretive explanation and not prediction".

This suggests that it may not be necessary to locate every available study because, for example, the results of a conceptual synthesis will not change if ten rather than five studies contain the same concept, but will depend on the range of concepts found in the studies, their context, and whether they are in agreement or not.

Thus, principles such as aiming for 'conceptual saturation' might be more appropriate when planning a search strategy for qualitative research, although it is not yet clear how these principles can be applied in practice. Similarly, other principles from primary qualitative research methods may also be 'borrowed' such as deliberately seeking studies which might act as negative cases, aiming for maximum variability and, in essence, designing the resulting set of studies to be heterogeneous, in some ways, instead of achieving the homogeneity that is often the aim in statistical meta-analyses.

How To Write A Synthesis Essay (Definition + Topics + Outline)

However you look, qualitative research is difficult to find [ 40 — 42 ]. In our review, it was not possible to rely on simple electronic searches of databases. We needed to search extensively in 'grey' literature, ask authors of relevant papers if they knew of more studies, and look especially for book chapters, and we spent a lot of effort screening titles and abstracts by hand and looking through journals manually.

In this sense, while we were not driven by the statistical imperative of locating every relevant study, when it actually came down to searching, we found that there was very little difference in the methods we had to use to find qualitative studies compared to the methods we use when searching for studies for inclusion in a meta-analysis. Assessing the quality of qualitative research has attracted much debate and there is little consensus regarding how quality should be assessed, who should assess quality, and, indeed, whether quality can or should be assessed in relation to 'qualitative' research at all [ 43 , 22 , 44 , 45 ].

We take the view that the quality of qualitative research should be assessed to avoid drawing unreliable conclusions. However, since there is little empirical evidence on which to base decisions for excluding studies based on quality assessment, we took the approach in this review to use 'sensitivity analyses' described below to assess the possible impact of study quality on the review's findings.

In our example review we assessed our studies according to 12 criteria, which were derived from existing sets of criteria proposed for assessing the quality of qualitative research [ 46 — 49 ], principles of good practice for conducting social research with children [ 50 ], and whether studies employed appropriate methods for addressing our review questions. The 12 criteria covered three main quality issues.

Literature Reviews: Synthesise & Write

Five related to the quality of the reporting of a study's aims, context, rationale, methods and findings e. A further four criteria related to the sufficiency of the strategies employed to establish the reliability and validity of data collection tools and methods of analysis, and hence the validity of the findings. The final three criteria related to the assessment of the appropriateness of the study methods for ensuring that findings about the barriers to, and facilitators of, healthy eating were rooted in children's own perspectives e.

One issue which is difficult to deal with when synthesising 'qualitative' studies is 'what counts as data' or 'findings'?

Pairing Bloom’s Taxonomy with other effective study strategies

This problem is easily addressed when a statistical meta-analysis is being conducted: the numeric results of RCTs — for example, the mean difference in outcome between the intervention and control — are taken from published reports and are entered into the software package being used to calculate the pooled effect size [ 3 , 51 ]. Deciding what to abstract from the published report of a 'qualitative' study is much more difficult. Campbell et al.

However, finding the key concepts in 'qualitative' research is not always straightforward either. As Sandelowski and Barroso [ 52 ] discovered, identifying the findings in qualitative research can be complicated by varied reporting styles or the misrepresentation of data as findings as for example when data are used to 'let participants speak for themselves'.

Sandelowski and Barroso [ 53 ] have argued that the findings of qualitative and, indeed, all empirical research are distinct from the data upon which they are based, the methods used to derive them, externally sourced data, and researchers' conclusions and implications.

In our example review, while it was relatively easy to identify 'data' in the studies — usually in the form of quotations from the children themselves — it was often difficult to identify key concepts or succinct summaries of findings, especially for studies that had undertaken relatively simple analyses and had not gone much further than describing and summarising what the children had said.

To resolve this problem we took study findings to be all of the text labelled as 'results' or 'findings' in study reports — though we also found 'findings' in the abstracts which were not always reported in the same way in the text. Study reports ranged in size from a few pages to full final project reports. We entered all the results of the studies verbatim into QSR's NVivo software for qualitative data analysis.

Where we had the documents in electronic form this process was straightforward even for large amounts of text. When electronic versions were not available, the results sections were either re-typed or scanned in using a flat-bed or pen scanner. We have since adapted our own reviewing system, 'EPPI-Reviewer' [ 54 ], to handle this type of synthesis and the screenshots below show this software. The synthesis took the form of three stages which overlapped to some degree: the free line-by-line coding of the findings of primary studies; the organisation of these 'free codes' into related areas to construct 'descriptive' themes; and the development of 'analytical' themes.

In our children and healthy eating review, we originally planned to extract and synthesise study findings according to our review questions regarding the barriers to, and facilitators of, healthy eating amongst children. It soon became apparent, however, that few study findings addressed these questions directly and it appeared that we were in danger of ending up with an empty synthesis. We were also concerned about imposing the a priori framework implied by our review questions onto study findings without allowing for the possibility that a different or modified framework may be a better fit.

We therefore temporarily put our review questions to one side and started from the study findings themselves to conduct an thematic analysis. There were eight relevant qualitative studies examining children's views of healthy eating. We entered the verbatim findings of these studies into our database. Three reviewers then independently coded each line of text according to its meaning and content.

Share This Book

Figure 1 illustrates this line-by-line coding using our specialist reviewing software, EPPI-Reviewer, which includes a component designed to support thematic synthesis. The text which was taken from the report of the primary study is on the left and codes were created inductively to capture the meaning and content of each sentence. Codes could be structured, either in a tree form as shown in the figure or as 'free' codes — without a hierarchical structure.

The use of line-by-line coding enabled us to undertake what has been described as one of the key tasks in the synthesis of qualitative research: the translation of concepts from one study to another [ 32 , 55 ]. However, this process may not be regarded as a simple one of translation. As we coded each new study we added to our 'bank' of codes and developed new ones when necessary. As well as translating concepts between studies, we had already begun the process of synthesis For another account of this process, see Doyle [[ 39 ], p]. Every sentence had at least one code applied, and most were categorised using several codes e.

Before completing this stage of the synthesis, we also examined all the text which had a given code applied to check consistency of interpretation and to see whether additional levels of coding were needed. In grounded theory this is termed 'axial' coding; see Fisher [ 55 ] for further discussion of the application of axial coding in research synthesis.

This process created a total of 36 initial codes. The adverts for unhealthy things tell you how nice they taste.

Learning to analyze and critically evaluate ideas, arguments, and points of view

Some children reported throwing away foods they knew had been put in because they were 'good for you' and only ate the crisps and chocolate. Reviewers looked for similarities and differences between the codes in order to start grouping them into a hierarchical tree structure. New codes were created to capture the meaning of groups of initial codes. This process resulted in a tree structure with several layers to organize a total of 12 descriptive themes Figure 2.


  • Background.
  • which essay writing service is the best in uk.
  • Four principles to make evidence synthesis more useful for policy.
  • Sections of This Topic Include.
  • Internet Resources.
  • persuasive essay on war on drugs.

For example, the first layer divided the 12 themes into whether they were concerned with children's understandings of healthy eating or influences on children's food choice. The above example, about children's preferences for food, was placed in both areas, since the findings related both to children's reactions to the foods they were given, and to how they behaved when given the choice over what foods they might eat.

A draft summary of the findings across the studies organized by the 12 descriptive themes was then written by one of the review authors. Two other review authors commented on this draft and a final version was agreed.

Analyzing, Interpreting and Reporting Basic Research Results

Up until this point, we had produced a synthesis which kept very close to the original findings of the included studies. The findings of each study had been combined into a whole via a listing of themes which described children's perspectives on healthy eating. However, we did not yet have a synthesis product that addressed directly the concerns of our review — regarding how to promote healthy eating, in particular fruit and vegetable intake, amongst children.

Neither had we 'gone beyond' the findings of the primary studies and generated additional concepts, understandings or hypotheses. As noted earlier, the idea or step of 'going beyond' the content of the original studies has been identified by some as the defining characteristic of synthesis [ 32 , 14 ]. This stage of a qualitative synthesis is the most difficult to describe and is, potentially, the most controversial, since it is dependent on the judgement and insights of the reviewers.

The equivalent stage in meta-ethnography is the development of 'third order interpretations' which go beyond the content of original studies [ 32 , 11 ]. In our example, the step of 'going beyond' the content of the original studies was achieved by using the descriptive themes that emerged from our inductive analysis of study findings to answer the review questions we had temporarily put to one side. Reviewers inferred barriers and facilitators from the views children were expressing about healthy eating or food in general, captured by the descriptive themes, and then considered the implications of children's views for intervention development.

Each reviewer first did this independently and then as a group. Through this discussion more abstract or analytical themes began to emerge. The barriers and facilitators and implications for intervention development were examined again in light of these themes and changes made as necessary.

Publication details

For example, five of the 12 descriptive themes concerned the influences on children's choice of foods food preferences, perceptions of health benefits, knowledge behaviour gap, roles and responsibilities, non-influencing factors. From these, reviewers inferred several barriers and implications for intervention development. Children identified readily that taste was the major concern for them when selecting food and that health was either a secondary factor or, in some cases, a reason for rejecting food.

Children also felt that buying healthy food was not a legitimate use of their pocket money, which they would use to buy sweets that could be enjoyed with friends.